Korean Society For Cognitive and Biological Psychology
[ Article ]
The Korean Journal of Cognitve & Biological Psychology - Vol. 30, No. 2, pp.141-156
ISSN: 1226-9654 (Print)
Print publication date 30 Apr 2018
Received 05 Mar 2018 Revised 29 Apr 2018 Accepted 30 Apr 2018
DOI: https://doi.org/10.22172/cogbio.2018.30.2.004

Perceiving causal relations between moving objects: Postdictive causal attribution can bias apparent motion correspondence

Sung-Ho Kim ; Ye-Eun Jung
Department of Psychology, Ewha Womans University
지각된 인과성이 애매한 가현운동 지각에 미치는 영향
김성호 ; 정예은
이화여자대학교 심리학과

Correspondence to: 김성호, 이화여자대학교 심리학과, (120-750) 서울시 서대문구 이화여대길 52 E-mail: sunghokim@ewha.ac.kr

Abstract

Comprehension of physical events in terms of cause and effect is fundamental for making sense of and dealing successfully with changes in the dynamic physical world. Previous research has demonstrated that the causal structure of the world can, in some cases, be directly perceived: When two billiard balls collide, observers perceive that the action of one ball caused the other's motion, merging two motion events into a unitary percept. The current study explored whether such casual interpretations can contribute to resolving low-level ambiguities in motion perception. We used a bistable apparent motion display, a motion quartet, which can lead to the perception of either horizontal or vertical motion, and tested the effects of “context objects” which moved in such a way that motion targets appeared to collide with them in either horizontal or vertical dimension. Our results show that contextual motion implying a Michotte-style launch can strongly bias observed motion correspondence, consistent with physical regularities of mechanical causality in a postdictive way. It suggests that the perception of causality is an earlier and more pervasive phenomenon than previously understood, and in fact can influence the perception of motion itself.

초록

물리적 세계의 역동적 변화를 이해하기 위해서는 물리적 사건을 인과적 관계로 해석하는 것이 필요하다. 인과성 지각에 대한 선행 연구는 세상의 인과적 구조가 때로는 직접적으로 지각가능함을 보여준다. 본 연구는 수직 혹은 수평 움직임으로 지각 가능한 "네 물체 움직임" 패러다임을 이용하여, 지각된 인과성이 애매한 가현운동 지각에 미치는 영향을 살펴보았다. 가상 사각형의 두 대각 귀퉁이에 번갈아 제시되는 가현운동 표적 물체가 두 쌍의 맥락 물체와 함께 제시되었다. 각 쌍의 표적 물체가 나타날 때마다 인접한 한 쌍의 맥락 물체가 수직 또는 수평 방향으로 이동하면, 물체 간 “충돌” 해석과 일관되는 방향으로 표적 물체의 움직임이 경험되는 편향이 관찰되었다. 본 연구는 (1)인과성 지각이 가현운동의 대응 문제 해결에 기여할 수 있으며, (2)시간 순서상 후행사건인 맥락 물체의 이동이 선행사건인 표적 물체의 움직임 방향에 영향을 주는 사후해석적인 방식으로 가현운동이 지각됨을 보여준다. 이 현상은 인과성 지각이 물체 움직임 정보를 기초로 한 일방향적 시각 처리과정의 최종 단계가 아니며, 기초적인 운동 지각과정에 하향적으로 기여할 수 있음을 시사한다.

Keywords:

causal perception, apparent motion, motion correspondence, postdiction

키워드:

인과적 지각, 가현운동, 움직임 대응문제, 사후추정

References

  • Anstis, S., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1987). Visual inertia in apparent motion. Vision Research, 27, 755-764. [https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(87)90073-3]
  • Bechlivanidis, C., & Lagnado, D. A. (2016). Time reordered: Causal perception guides the interpretation of temporal order. Cognition, 146, 58-66. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.001]
  • Boyle, D. (1960). A contribution to the study of phenomenal causation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 171–179. [https://doi.org/10.1080/17470216008416721]
  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). Psychophysics software for use with MATLAB. Spatial Vision, 10, 433-436. [https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357]
  • Buehner, M., & Humphreys, G. (2010). Causal contraction: Spatial binding in the perception of collision events. Psychological Science, 21, 44-48. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609354735]
  • Chaudhuri, A., & Glaser, D. A. (1991). Metastable motion anisotropy. Visual neuroscience, 7, 397-407. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523800009706]
  • Choi, H., & Scholl, B. J. (2004). Effects of grouping and attention on the perception of causality. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 926–942. [https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194985]
  • Choi, H., & Scholl, B. J. (2006). Perceiving causality after the fact: Postdiction in the temporal dynamics of causal perception. Perception, 35, 385. [https://doi.org/10.1068/p5462]
  • Gordon, I., Day, R., & Stecher, E. (1990). Perceived causality occurs with stroboscopic movement of one or both stimulus elements. Perception, 19, 17–20. [https://doi.org/10.1068/p190017]
  • Hock, H. S., Kelso, J. S., & Schöner, G. (1993). Bistability and hysteresis in the organization of apparent motion patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 63-80. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.19.1.63]
  • Kim, S. H., Feldman, J., & Singh, M. (2013). Perceived causality can alter the perceived trajectory of apparent motion. Psychological science, 24, 575-582. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612458529]
  • Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality (T. R. Miles & E. Miles, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1946).
  • Natsoulas, T. (1961). Principles of momentum and kinetic energy in the perception of causality. American Journal of Psychology, 74, 394–402. [https://doi.org/10.2307/1419745]
  • Pantle, A. J., Gallogly, D. P., & Piehler, O. C. (2000). Direction biasing by brief apparent motion stimuli. Vision Research, 40, 1979–1991. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00071-7]
  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442. [https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00366]
  • Pinkus, A., & Pantle, A. (1997). Probing visual motion signals with a priming paradigm. Vision Research, 37, 541–552. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00162-9]
  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Anstis, S. M. (1983). Perceptual organization in moving patterns. Nature. 304, 529–531. [https://doi.org/10.1038/304529a0]
  • Rolfs, M., Dambacher, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2013). Visual adaptation of the perception of causality. Current Biology, 23, 250-254. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.017]
  • Schlottmann, A., & Anderson, N. H. (1993). An information integration approach to phenomenal causality. Memory & Cognition, 21, 201–785. [https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202746]
  • Schlottmann, A., & Shanks, D. (1992). Evidence for a distance between judged and perceived causality. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 321–342. [https://doi.org/10.1080/02724989243000055]
  • Scholl, B. J., & Gao, T. (2013). Perceiving animacy and intentionality: Visual processing or higher-level judgment? In M. D. Rutherford & V. A. Kuhlmeier (Eds.), Social perception: Detection and interpretation of animacy, agency, and intention (pp. 197-230). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262019279.003.0009]
  • Scholl, B. J., & Nakayama, K. (2002). Causal capture: Contextual effects on the perception of collision events. Psychological Science, 13, 493–498. [https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00487]
  • Scholl, B. J., & Nakayama, K. (2004). Illusory causal crescents: Misperceived spatial relations due to perceived causality. Perception, 33, 455-470. [https://doi.org/10.1068/p5172]
  • Scholl, B. J., & Tremoulet, P. D. (2000). Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 299–309. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01506-0]
  • Shimojo, S. & Nakayama, K. (1990). Amodal presence of partially occluded surfaces: role of invisible stimuli in apparent motion correspondence. Perception, 19, 285-299. [https://doi.org/10.1068/p190285]
  • Ullman, S. (1979). The interpretation of visual motion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Wertheimer, M. (1961). Experimental studies on the seeing of motion. In T. Shipley (Ed.), Classics in psychology (pp. 1032-1088). New York: Philosophical Library. (Original work published 1912).
  • Wichmann, F. A., & Hill, N. J. (2001a). The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 63, 1293-1313. [https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194544]
  • Wichmann, F. A., & Hill, N. J. (2001b). The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 1314–1329. [https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194545]
  • Zacks, J. M., & Tversky, B. (2001). Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological bulletin, 127, 3-21. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.3]